"Her"
The power of this fusion fantasy can be seen in a simple experiment that has been repeated over and over again by Silverman and his group. They showed subliminal messages to hundreds of people, and found that only one—"MOMMY AND I ARE ONE”—had an enormous emotional effect, reducing their anxieties and pathologies and their smoking and drinking addictions measurably. “Daddy and I are one” had no effect.
"Iron Man 3"
Warriors become fused with the powerful mother that masturbated them during menstruation; they then decorate themselves with menstrual blood-red paint so they can appropriate the fearful power of their Killer Mothers.
Wars in early
civilizations are fought on behalf of and against Killer Goddesses,
bloodthirsty mothers like Tiamat, Ishtar, Inanna, Isis or Kali. Typical is the Aztec mother-goddess
Hiutzilopochtli, who had “mouths all over her body” that cried out to be fed
the blood of soldiers. Scholars of antiquity conclude: “The oldest deities of warfare and
destruction were feminine, not masculine.” Jungian
analysts called her the
Terrible Mother archetype, a Dragon-Mother with “a mouth bristling with teeth…so that it may
devour us.” Ovid captures the mother of antiquity by picturing Pentheus crying out “Oh
Mother, gaze at me! She screamed at him, and shook her flying
hair. Then Agave ripped his head from fallen shoulders, raised it up [and] cried, ‘Here is my work, my victory.’”
That wars and sacrifices also
act out the child’s revenge against the mother can be seen in the
details of the sacrifice of women (about a third of all the sacrifices), where female
victims first make a prodigious show of their female power, then are laid down on their
backs and their breasts cut open and their bodies torn apart. The
two aspects of the Killer Goddess are demonstrated when the Aztec warrior takes the sword that he
had used to behead the Goddess victim and “terrifies and annihilates our enemies with it.
"Gravity"
Furthermore, the weight of the fetus pressing down into the pelvis can compress blood vessels supplying the placenta, producing additional placental failure. Practice contractions near birth give the fetus periodic "squeezes," decreasing oxygen level even further, while birth itself is so hypoxic that "hypoxia of a certain degree and duration is a normal phenomenon in every delivery," not just in more severe cases. The effects on the fetus of this extreme hypoxia are dramatic: normal fetal breathing stops, fetal heart rate accelerates, then decelerates, and the fetus thrashes about frantically in a life-and death struggle to liberate itself from its terrifying asphyxiation.
Furthermore, the weight of the fetus pressing down into the pelvis can compress blood vessels supplying the placenta, producing additional placental failure. Practice contractions near birth give the fetus periodic "squeezes," decreasing oxygen level even further, while birth itself is so hypoxic that "hypoxia of a certain degree and duration is a normal phenomenon in every delivery," not just in more severe cases. The effects on the fetus of this extreme hypoxia are dramatic: normal fetal breathing stops, fetal heart rate accelerates, then decelerates, and the fetus thrashes about frantically in a life-and death struggle to liberate itself from its terrifying asphyxiation.
It is one of the most basic
principles of psychoanalysis that massive quantities of stimulation,
particularly intensely painful experiences, result in a severe
"trauma" for the individual, particularly when the ego is too
immature to prevent itself from being overwhelmed by the affects. That fetal
distress is traumatic can hardly be doubted, as the fetus has as yet none of
the psychological defense mechanisms to handle massive anxiety and rage.
Therefore, as psychoanalysts long ago found true of all traumatizations-from
early enema-giving to war-time shocks or concentration camp experiences-the
psyche then needs to endlessly re-experience the trauma in a specific
"repetition compulsion" which, as Greenacre first pointed out, is
similar to "imprinting" in lower animals. As no psychic apparatus is
as open to trauma as that of the helpless fetus, no repetition compulsion is as
strong as that which results from the "imprinting" of the fetal drama
of repeated feelings of asphyxiation, blood pollution, and cleansing, climaxed
by a cataclysmic battle and a liberation through a painful birth process.
Although the form that this endlessly repeated death-and-rebirth fetal drama
takes in later life is determined by the kind of childrearing which is
experienced, the basic "imprinted" fetal drama can nevertheless
always be discovered behind all the other overlays, pre-oedioal or oedipal.
The "imprinted" fetal drama, then, is the
matrix into which is poured all later childhood experiences, as the child works
over the basic questions posed by his experiences in the womb: Is the world
hopelessly divided between nurturant and poisonous objects? Am I to be
eternally helpless and dependent on the life-giving blood of others? Must all
good feelings be interrupted by painful ones? Do I always have to battle for
every pleasure? Will I have the support and room I need to grow? Can one ever
really rely on another? Is entropy the law of my world, with everything doomed
to get more crowded and polluted? Must I spend my life endlessly killing
enemies?
"12 Years a Slave"
It is only when one
realizes that we all carry around with us persecutory social alters that become manifest in groups that such unexplained experiments as those described in Stanley
Milgram's classic study Obedience to Authority become understandable. In
this experiment, people were asked to be "teachers" and, whenever their
"learners" made mistakes, to give them massive electric shocks. The "learners,"
who were only acting the part, were trained to give out pained cries even though the
"electric shocks" were non-existent. Of the 40 "teachers,"
65 percent delivered the maximum
amount of shock even as they watched the "learners" scream out in pain and plead
to be released, despite their having been told they didn't have to step up the shock
level. The "teachers" often trembled, groaned and were extremely upset at
having to inflict the painful shocks, but continued to do so nonetheless. That the
"teachers" believed the shocks were real is confirmed by another version of
the experiment in which real shocks were inflicted upon a little puppy, who howled in
protest; the obedience statistics were similar.
Social scientists
have been puzzled by Milgram's experiments, wondering why people were so easily
talked into inflicting pain so gratuitously. The real explanation is that, by joining a
group-the "university experiment"-they switched into their social alters and merged
with their own sadistic internalized persecutor, which was quite willing to take
responsibility for ordering pain inflicted upon others. Their "struggle with themselves"
over whether to obey was really a struggle between their social alters and their main
selves. Although many subsequent experiments varied the conditions forobedience,
what Milgram did not do is try the experiment without the social trance. If he had not
framed it as a group experience, if he had simply on his own authority walked up
to each individual, alone, and, without alluding to a university or any other group,
asked him or her to come to his home and give massive amounts of electric shock to
punish someone, he would not have been obeyed, because they would not have
switched into their social alters. The crucial element of the experiments was the
existence of the group-as-terrifying-parent, the all-powerful university. Not
surprisingly, when the experiment was repeated using children-who go into trance and
switch into traumatized content more easily than adults-they were even more obedient in
inflicting the maximum shock. Subjects
were even obedient when they themselves
were the victims: 54 percent turned a dial upon command to the maximum limit
when they had been told it was inflicting damage upon their ears that could lead to
their own deafness, and 74 percent ate food they thought could harm them, thus
confirming that they were truly in a dissociated state, not just "obeying"
authority or trying to hurt others, and that it was actually an alternate self doing the hurting of
the main self. The only time they refused to obey was
when experimenters pretended to act out a group rebellion, since the social trance was broken.
Milgram
could also have tested whether it was simple obedience that was really being tested
by asking his subjects to reach into their pockets and pay some money to the
learners. They would have refused to do so, because they weren't "obeying"
any old command, they were using the experimental situation to hurt scapegoats.
"Filth"
"Filth"
The only
neurobiological condition inherited by boys that affects later violence is they have a
smaller corpus callosum, the part of the brain that connects the right and the left
hemisphere. The larger corpus
callosum of infant girls allows them to work through trauma
and neglect more easily than boys. Furthermore, boys who are abused had a 25
percent reduction in sections of the corpus callosum, while girls did not. This means boys actually need more love
and caretaking than girls as they grow up. If they do
not receive enough interpersonal attention from their caretakers they suffer from
damaged prefrontal cortices (self control, empathy) and from hyperactive amygdalae
(fear centers), their corpus callosum is reduced further, and they have reduced
serotonin levels (calming ability) and increased corticosterone production (stress
hormone). All these factors make them have weak selves, reduced empathy, less control over impulsive violence and far more
fears than girls.
The central
psychobiological question, then, is this: Are boys given more love and attention than
girls by their caretakers in order to help them offset their greater needs? The answer, of course, is just the opposite: boys are given less care and support, from
everyone in the family and in society, and they are abused far more than girls, so by the
time they are three years of age they become twice as violent as girls. Boys’ greater violence by this time, including their propensity to form dominance gangs and to endlessly “play war,” are the results of their greater abuse and distancing by adults and
being subject to demands to “grow up” and “be manly” and “not be a crybaby” and not
need attachment —attitudes taught by their parents, teachers and coaches. By age four boys’ play is full of provocations that test their selfworth: “At 4 years
of age, girls’ insults to one another are infrequent and minor…Boy/boy insults, however,
are numerous and tough.” The so-called “aggressiveness” usually
ascribed to boys is in fact wholly defensive, as they try to ward off their
greater feelings of insecurity and hopelessness. It isn’t “aggression” males display; it’s bravado—defensive
testing and disproof of their fears.
The mother, of
course, is the focal point of this widespread distancing and insecure attachment pattern.
High levels of violence and of testosterone have been shown to be associated with poorer relationships with mothers, not fathers, since mothers are the primary
caretakers in most families (even in America today, fathers spend only an average of eleven
minutes a day with their children). It
is not just genetics but more importantly
maternal environment that Tronick and Weinberg blame when they see from their studies
that “Infant boys are more emotionally reactive than girls. They display more
positive as well as negative affect, focus more on the mother, and display more signals
expressing escape and distress and demands for contact than do girls.” This is because from infancy boys are expected to
“just grow up” and not need as much
emotional care as girls—indeed, boys are regularly encouraged
not to express any of their feelings,
since this is seen as “weak” or “babyish” in boys. While mothers may
sometimes dominate their little girls and expect them to share their emotional problems, they distance their boys by not making contact with them and expect them to “be a man.”
This begins from birth: “Over the first three months of life, a baby girl’s
skills in eye contact and mutual facial gazing will increase by over 400 percent, whereas facial gazing skills in a boy during this time will not increase at all.” Boys grow up with less attachment
strengths because careful studies show that
mothers look at their boys less, because both parents hit their boys two or three times as
much as they do their girls, because boys are at much higher risk than girls for
serious violence against them, and because boys are continuously told to be “tough,” not to be a
“wimp” or a “weakling,” not to be “soft” or a “sissy.” As Tom Brown told his chum when
he wanted him to appear more manly: “Don’t ever talk about home, or your
mother and sisters…you’ll get bullied.” Real boys don’t admit they need their mothers.
When William Pollack researched his book Real Boys’ Voices, he asked boys “Have
you ever been called a ‘wuss,’ ‘wimp,’ or ‘fag’? ‘Oh, that,’ one boy said. ‘That happens every day. I thought it was just a part of being a boy!’” Another said, “Boys
are just as sensitive as girls are, but we’re not allowed to show our feelings. We’re put in
this narrow box and if we try to break out, we’re made fun of, or threatened.’” Pollack accurately shows boys are not
more “aggressive”—they are just more often shamed if they show their feelings. He accurately says “bravado is a
defense against shame we too often mistake for ‘badness’ what is really covert
sadness and frustration about having to fulfill an impossible test of self.” This intense sadness and rage at being
abandoned is deeply unconscious, dissociated—what Garbarino terms “the emotional amnesia of lost boys.”
But the crucial
variable is the distancing and lack of care given to boys by most mothers in all
societies. Whether it is because mothers are female and can more closely identify with
the needs of their girls or because the boys are male like their husbands and are blamed for their failings and lack of help in child care or any one of dozens of other
reasons that we will examine in the next chapter, mothers teach their boys that “it is not
enough to separate from her; he must make a total, wrenching split [and]
exorcise any aspect of his mother from his own personality….The battle between
establishing distance and clinging to dependence takes hold of a boy
almost at the moment that he learns to differentiate himself from his mother or sister
as a male, rather than a female.” The
only way boys sometimes are allowed to get
close to their mothers is when they are sick—times that are remembered by men as
blissful since only then can they admit their desperate need for nurturing. In contrast, “over
80 percent of the men in my study remembered a recurring childhood
nightmare of coming home from school and finding their mothers gone. With
mounting terror, the little boy would run from room to room looking for his mother…most of
the men described memories of a deep loneliness, feelings of being totally helpless.”
Texts
"Foundations of Psychohistory"
"Emotional Life of Nations"
"The Origins of War in Child Abuse"
Texts
"Foundations of Psychohistory"
"Emotional Life of Nations"
"The Origins of War in Child Abuse"
Comments
Post a Comment